Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Cumulative exposure to high-strain and active jobs as predictors of cognitive function: the Whitehall II study
  1. M Elovainio1,2,
  2. J E Ferrie1,
  3. A Singh-Manoux1,3,
  4. D Gimeno1,
  5. R De Vogli1,
  6. M J Shipley1,
  7. J Vahtera4,
  8. E J Brunner1,
  9. M G Marmot1,
  10. M Kivimäki1,4
  1. 1
    University College London, London, UK
  2. 2
    National Research and Development Centre for Welfare and Health, Helsinki, Finland
  3. 3
    INSERM U697-IFR69, Paris, France
  4. 4
    Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Helsinki, Finland
  1. Marko Elovainio, National Research and Development Centre for Welfare and Health, PO Box 220, Fi-00370 Helsinki, Finland; marko.elovainio{at}stakes.fi

Abstract

Objectives: A high-strain job (a combination of high job demands and low job control) is expected to increase the risk of health problems, whereas an active job (high demands and high control) can be hypothesised to be associated with a greater capacity to learn. We tested associations between high-strain and active jobs and cognitive function in middle-aged men and women.

Methods: Data on 4146 British civil servants (2989 men and 1157 women) aged 35–55 years at baseline came from the Whitehall II study. Cumulative exposure to both high-strain and active jobs was assessed at phases 1 (1985–1988), 2 (1989–1990) and 3 (1991–1993). Cognitive performance was assessed at phases 5 (1997–1999) and 7 (2003–2004) using the following tests: verbal memory, inductive reasoning (Alice Heim), verbal meaning (Mill Hill), phonemic and semantic fluency. Analyses were adjusted for age, sex and employment grade.

Results: Longer exposure to high job strain and shorter exposure to active jobs were associated with lower scores in most of the cognitive performance tests. However, these associations disappeared on adjustment for employment grade. Phonemic fluency was an exception to this pattern. Associations between exposure to an active job and phonemic fluency at both follow-up phases were robust to adjustment for employment grade. However, there was no association between exposure to active jobs and change in phonemic fluency score between the follow-up phases after adjustment for employment grade.

Conclusions: In these data, associations between cumulative exposure to high-strain or active jobs and cognition are largely explained by socioeconomic position.

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Main messages

Very few studies have tested the fundamental learning hypothesis of the job demand-control model.

Our results did not offer strong support for the cumulative learning possibilities and following cognitive capacity of active work.

High-strain work did not seem to be a strong predictor of low learning capacity measured by cognitive tests.

Policy implications

Further research is needed to determine the possible interventions in reducing socioeconomic inequalities in learning capacity in terms of cognitive functions.

The job strain model (also known as the demand-control model) proposes that high-strain jobs, a combination of high job demands and low job control, are hazardous to employee health. This proposition has received extensive endorsement in the literature.18 The model also suggests that high strain inhibits learning and may thus have an adverse effect on cognitive capabilities in the long run. Another fundamental, but less explored hypothesis relates to active jobs. These jobs combine the chance to control the main parameters of one’s job with high and challenging job demands. It is possible that active jobs contribute to learning new skills and/or coping strategies which9 10 in turn, may improve cognitive function and protect against cognitive decline. Yet, to our knowledge, only two longitudinal studies have assessed the association between components of the job strain model and cognitive functioning. One found that learning-like outcomes such as self-efficacy mediated the effect of job control on depression and the authors concluded that high job strain inhibits learning.11 Another study found that high levels of job strain had adverse effects on learning among new employees.12 Both studies were based on small samples, short follow-up periods and self-reported outcomes.

Adverse effects on learning and memory, following intense stress have repeatedly been found.1317 It has been reported, for instance, that intense stress results in marked inaccuracies in memory.1820 Also chronic stress has been shown to affect memory and other cognitive functions. Öhman and others21 found performance deficits for episodic memory and for tasks requiring divided attention during either encoding or retrieval of words among chronic stress patients. Poor performance was also found in mental tempo, prospective memory and semantic access.

In this study, we examined whether shorter exposure to active jobs and longer exposure to high job strain was associated with poorer cognitive function in the Whitehall ll study, a cohort of British civil servants. The advantage of using Whitehall ll data is the opportunity to examine cumulative exposure to high-strain and active jobs as predictors of cognitive function in a large, longitudinal occupational cohort with a wide range of cognitive function measures, such as memory functions, inductive reasoning, verbal meaning, semantic fluency and phonemic fluency.

METHODS

Participants and design

The target population for the Whitehall II study was all office staff aged 35–55 years based in 20 civil service departments in London, UK. With a 73% participation rate, the cohort included 6895 men and 3413 women at study entry in 1985–1988. The present study focused on the 2989 men (43% of all men participants) and 1157 women (34% of all women participants) who responded to a survey on job strain at phases 1 (the baseline of this study, 1985–1988), 2 (1989–1990) and 3 (1991–1993), and completed the cognitive function test at phases 5 (1997–99) and 7 (2003–2004).

High-strain jobs and active jobs

Components of the job strain model were measured using the Job Strain Questionnaire.22 Job demands were measured using four items (internal consistency, Cronbach’s α = 0.67), and job control using 15 items (internal consistency, Cronbach’s α = 0.84). All questions were answered on a four-point scale from “often” to “never/almost never”. Responses were combined and then divided into high and low based on the median split for the respective component. Four job categories were created based on the demand-control model: active (high control and high demands), low strain (high control and low demands), passive (low control and low demands) and high strain (low control and high demands).23 The accumulation of exposure to high-strain and active jobs over the three measurement periods (phases 1, 2, and 3) was computed by adding together the number of times the participant had been working in high-strain or active jobs.

Cognitive function

Cognitive data are drawn from phases 5 and 7, when cognitive testing was performed on all participants attending the Whitehall ll medical examination. Cognitive function was measured using the following five standard tests:

(a) Verbal memory was assessed by a 20-word free recall test of short-term memory. Participants were presented a list of 20 one- or two-syllable words at 2-second intervals and were then asked to recall in writing as many of the words in any order within 2 minutes.

(b) The Alice Heim intelligence test (AH4)24 is composed of a series of 65 items (32 verbal and 33 mathematical reasoning items) of increasing difficulty. This is a test of inductive reasoning that measures the ability to identify patterns and infer principles and rules. Participants had 10 minutes to complete this section.

(c) The Mill Hill vocabulary test25 assesses knowledge of verbal meaning and encompasses the ability to recognise and comprehend words. We used the test in its multiple format, which consists of a list of 33 stimulus words ordered by increasing difficulty, and six response choices per word.

(d) Two measures of verbal fluency: phonemic and semantic, were used.26 Phonemic fluency was assessed via “S” words and semantic fluency via “animal” words. Subjects were asked to recall in writing as many words beginning with “S” and as many animal names as they could. One minute was allowed for each test. High scores on all tests denote better performance.

Change scores in cognitive function between phases 5 and 7 were defined by taking the difference in the cognitive function score between phases 5 and 7. As previously,27 28 decline was defined as performance in the worst quintile of change. Because the interval between phases 5 and 7 was not equal for everybody (mean 5.4 years, range 3.2 to 7.2 years) further adjustments were made by including the time interval as a covariate in the regression models.

Potential confounding factors

Potential confounding factors measured at phase 5 included sex, age, employment grade, smoking, alcohol consumption, body mass index (BMI), depression and hypertension. Socioeconomic position was measured as civil service employment grade (administrative, professional, clerical/support). Health behaviour measures were smoking (self-reported cigarette smoking classified as never smoker, former smoker and current smoker), alcohol consumption (units/week, classified as: none, 1–14 units, 15–21 units, 22+ units with the highest two categories combined in women), and body mass index (BMI, kg/m2, classified as under 18.5, 18.5−25, 25−30, over 30). Depressive symptoms in the Whitehall II study were measured using a four-item depression subscale (Cronbach α = 0.88). Derived from the General Health Questionnaire-30 (GHQ-30), the subscale was based on principal component analysis and compared with the depression subscale items from the GHQ-28.The four items requested whether the participant had recently:

(a) been thinking of him/herself as a worthless person;

(b) felt that life is entirely hopeless;

(c) felt that life isn’t worth living;

(d) found at times he/she couldn’t do anything because his/her nerves were too bad.

Response options were on a Likert scale from 0 to 3 (“not at all”, “no more than usual”, “rather more than usual”, and “much more than usual”).29 The items were summed up and those scoring 3 or more were classified as depressive. Subjects with systolic blood pressure (SBP) ⩾140 mmHg and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) ⩾90 mmHg or on antihypertensive treatment were classified as hypertensive.30

Statistical analysis

Relationships between cumulative exposure to (1) a high-strain job; and (2) an active job and cognitive function at phases 5 and 7 were analysed using least squares regression with continuous measures of the cognitive tests as the outcome. Least squares means were used to present the age-adjusted cognitive function means from these analyses. We additionally tested the associations using repeated analyses of variance. Statistical models were sequentially adjusted for age, sex and employment grade, and then additionally for alcohol consumption, smoking status, BMI, physical activity, depression and hypertension. Most of these factors have been shown to be associated with cognitive functioning previously.27 3133 Associations between cumulative exposure to (a) a high-strain job; and (b) an active job and subsequent change in cognitive function between phases 5 and 7 were examined using logistic regression with the worst quintile of change as the outcome. In comparing included participants with those excluded, a t test for the continuous variables and a chi-squared test for the categorical variables were used. The statistical tests were performed using Statistical Analysis System (SAS) version 9.01, and statistical significance was inferred from a 2-tailed p value <0.05.

RESULTS

Participants who were excluded from the current analyses were, at phase 1, older (45.1 years compared to 43.4 years, p<0.001), more likely to be women (38% vs 28%, p<0.001), and from the lowest employment grade (24% vs 11%, p<0.001). Additionally they had both higher job demands (mean 59.1 vs 56.7, p = 0.043) and job control level (mean 67.0 vs 63.1, p<0.001) at baseline.

Characteristics of the study participants at phase 5 are shown in table 1. Their mean age was 54.8 years; the majority worked in the highest grade. Only 8% were current smokers, less than one-fifth were classified as depressed and 39% were hypertensive. Four per cent of the participants were in high-strain jobs and 16% in active jobs at phases 1, 2 and 3. There was a slight decline in the average test score for each cognitive function measure between phases 5 and 7.

Table 1 Sample characteristics (n = 4146)*

Mean scores for the cognitive function measures at phases 5 and 7 by exposure to high-strain jobs are shown in table 2. Although the age- and sex-adjusted analyses showed declines in cognitive function with cumulative exposure to high-strain jobs, further adjustment for employment grade attenuated all the associations such that cumulative exposure to a high-strain job was not associated with any of the cognitive function tests at phases 5 or 7. Adjusting for smoking, alcohol consumption, BMI, depression and hypertension did not change the results and repeated analyses of variance did not produce any statistically significant exposure X time interactions (not shown).

Table 2 Association between high-strain job and cognitive functions at phases 5 and 7 (means and 95% CI)

Associations between exposure to an active job and cognitive function are presented in table 3. Increasing exposure to an active job over phases 1, 2 and 3 was associated with higher performance on all the cognitive function tests at phase 5 in analyses adjusted for age and sex. However, only the associations with verbal meaning at phase 5 and phonemic fluency at phases 5 and 7 were robust to additional adjustment for employment grade. Adjusting for smoking, alcohol consumption, BMI, depression and hypertension had no effect on these associations. There were no statistically significant exposure X time interactions between active job and phonemic fluency (not shown).

Table 3 Association between cumulative exposure to active job and cognitive functions at phases 5 and 7 (means and 95% CI)

Further analyses of change in cognitive function test scores between phases 5 and 7 were conducted only for verbal meaning and phonemic fluency as these were the measures associated with an active job (table 3, Model 2). As can be seen in table 4, there was an association between a high-strain job, and an inverse association between exposure to an active job, and a declining verbal meaning score between phases 5 and 7. However, these associations were not robust to adjustment for employment grade. Correspondingly, the inverse associations observed between an active job and phonemic fluency attenuated after adjustment for employment grade and did not remain statistically significant.

Table 4 Association between exposure to high-strain or active job during three study phases and verbal meaning and phonemic fluency change score (belonging to the worst quintile) between phases 5 and 7 (odds ratios (ORs) and 95% CI)

Because it is possible that the excluded subjects represented a more susceptible population (eg, lower cognitive reserve), the restriction criteria might have introduced attrition bias into our analyses. In order to rule out that possibility, we additionally tested the association between baseline exposure to high-strain or active jobs and cognitive functions at both study phases. Exposure to a high-strain job at baseline was associated with a lower verbal meaning score (Mill Hill) score at phase 5 (p = 0.002) and at phase 7 (p = 0.019) when adjusted for employment grade. None of the other tested associations were statistically significant.

DISCUSSION

In this study of a middle-aged population there were dose-response associations between cumulative exposure to high-strain or active jobs and several cognitive performance test scores before but not after adjustment for socioeconomic position. Only the association between an active job and phonemic fluency was robust to adjustment for employment grade at both follow-up periods which were, on average, 6 and 12 years after assessment of the exposure. However, no independent relationships were found between cumulative exposure to high-strain or active jobs and change in cognitive performance between the follow-ups.

Numerous studies have observed adverse effects on learning and memory following intense stress or glucocorticoid treatment.1317 The release of stress hormones, for example, epinephrine and glucocorticoids from the adrenal glands during or after emotionally stressful experiences, has been shown to affect the consolidation of lasting memories.18 Additionally, extensive evidence suggests that the amygdalae, and specifically the basolateral amygdalae, mediate stress hormone effects on memory functions.19 In previous investigations it has also been reported that chronic stress results in poor performance in many cognitive tests.21

Chronic psychological stress would have been a plausible explanation of our findings had we observed an association between high job strain and low cognition function. The failure to observe such an association in the present study suggests that high job strain may be a psychological stressor of insufficient intensity to result in long-term impairment of cognitive function. Indeed, the adverse effect of high job strain on cardiovascular outcomes is expected to result from long-lasting exposure to moderate- rather than high-intensity stress.34

In the few previous studies on this issue, it has been suggested that high-strain jobs inhibit learning,12 or have adverse effects on learning-like outcomes.11 Several issues may explain why our results are not in line with these longitudinal studies. First, our findings were based on a cohort of over 4000 employees whereas previous studies had much smaller samples. Second, our study is the only one to use cumulative exposure to the job strain categories to determine the long-term effects of work environment stressors. Although repeat measures may result in a more reliable assessment, there is a possibility that the effect of exposure to high-strain or active jobs may wear off due to habituation. A considerable proportion of the Whitehall ll participants had already retired from the civil service by phases 5 and 7 and so were no longer exposed to the negative effects of a high-strain job or the benefits of an active job, a factor that may lead to “dilution” of the effects.

We used a large variety of cognitive tests to determine learning outcomes. Previous studies have used self-determined or perceived sense of learning, such as sense of mastery and self-efficacy. These measures may be open to common-method bias that can artificially inflate associations. Furthermore, such self-assessment scales may reflect coping rather than cognition. Indeed, people may learn how to master or to cope with the demands of their job without any improvement in general cognitive capacity.

The nearly ubiquitous associations observed between high-strain or active jobs and cognitive performance in this study, were substantially reduced after adjustment for employment grade. A clear socioeconomic gradient has been found in both components of high-strain and active jobs. High job control and high demands are more common in higher socioeconomic groups.35 It is possible that the relationship between an active job in the high employment grades and cognitive performance is reciprocal. That is, active jobs may lead to better cognitive functioning in people who get the better jobs, that is, those with high psychological demands combined with high job control. While this cannot be ruled out, socioeconomic position is also a marker of myriad other risk factors that may affect cognition, such as early life conditions, education, and cardiovascular diseases.

Study strengths and weaknesses

This study has certain strengths that reduce the possibility of false-negative conclusions. The study benefits from using data from Whitehall II, a well-characterised cohort with sufficient power to detect effects, and repeated measures of the key factors: job strain components and cognitive function tests. Furthermore, cognitive function was tested using a wide variety of well-characterised validated tests. Nonetheless, it is important to note some limitations. First, the final analyses were conducted using participants with complete data on cognitive function at phases 5 and 7 and work characteristics at phases 1, 2 and 3. This meant that more than half the original population was excluded. There were some differences in baseline characteristics between included and excluded civil servants even in the exposure variables suggesting over representation of those belonging to the active category. Although selective processes may have resulted in some underestimation of the association between the job strain categories and cognitive function, they are unlikely to have completely masked an association. Second, our analysis of change in cognitive scores is based on only two waves of data. It is recognised that such analyses are subject to ceiling and floor effects, regression to the mean, and measurement error. However, data from two waves are a distinct improvement on cross-sectional analysis even though robust causal inferences are best obtained from multi-wave studies.36 Third, our data come from a cohort of older, white-collar civil servants and cannot be assumed to represent the general population. However, participants cover a good proportion of the socioeconomic spectrum. Given the increased representation of workers employed in white-collar occupations, our sample may be more representative than initially thought. In addition, the longitudinal changes in cognitive function measures between phases 5 and 7 were small, probably partly as a result of the relatively short follow-up time, limiting the statistical power to detect significant associations. However, there were statistically significant associations between exposure to an active job and two of the cognitive function measures, but those associations were not robust to adjustments for employment grade.

Conclusions

Although we found an association between active job and some measures of cognition, verbal meaning and verbal fluency, there was no evidence that an active job predicts change in these measures, independent of socioeconomic position. Our results do not offer strong support for the hypothesis that active work is causally associated, through cumulative learning experiences, with enhanced cognitive function or lower risk of cognitive decline. We also failed to obtain consistent support for job strain as a determinant of cognitive function.

Acknowledgments

The Whitehall II study has been supported by grants from the Medical Research Council; British Heart Foundation; Health and Safety Executive; Department of Health; National Heart Lung and Blood Institute (HL36310), US, NIH: National Institute on Aging (AG13196), US, NIH; Agency for Health Care Policy Research (HS06516); and the John D and Catherine T MacArthur Foundation Research Networks on Successful Midlife Development and Socio-economic Status and Health. J.E.F. is supported by the MRC (Grant number G8802774), M.J.S. by a grant from the British Heart Foundation, M.G.M. by an MRC Research Professorship, MK by the Academy of Finland (project 117604, 124322 and 124271), ME by the WEF (project 203533) and A S-M by EURYI award from the European Science Foundation. We thank all participating Civil Service departments and their welfare, personnel, and establishment officers; the Occupational Health and Safety Agency; the Council of Civil Service Unions; all participating civil servants in the Whitehall II study; all members of the Whitehall II study team.

REFERENCES

Footnotes

  • Competing interests: None.